Running Head: PHONOLOGICAL SIMILARITY Phonological similarity and Trace Degradation in the Serial Recall Task: When CAT helps RAT, but not MAN

نویسندگان

  • Anthony B. Fallon
  • Kim Groves
  • Gerald Tehan
  • Gerry Tehan
چکیده

Phonological similarity is observed to detrimentally affect serial recall when correctin-position scoring is used. Two experiments investigated the role of item and position accuracy scoring of rhyming, similar non-rhyming, and dissimilar lists under immediate recall conditions; articulatory suppression; or a filled delay. In general, rhyme lists produced the best item recall but position accuracy was highest for dissimilar. The results are due to a category cuing effect improving item recall for rhyme lists in conjunction with a detrimental effect of phonological similarity on position accuracy. Phonological similarity 4 Lists of words that sound similar to one another are harder to remember on an immediate serial recall task than lists of words that contain phonologically dissimilar items (Baddeley, 1966; Conrad & Hull, 1964). Most current models of immediate serial recall (Baddeley, 1986; Brown & Hulme, 1995; Burgess & Hitch, 1992; Nairne, 1990; Schweickert, 1993) assert that serial order is harder to maintain when all items in the list are phonologically similar to one another. In phonological similarity research, recall has most often been scored in terms of the number of items recalled in correct serial position. However, alternative scoring procedures are available. Item recall is usually scored by counting the number of list items recalled, regardless of the order in which they are recalled (e.g. Watkins, Watkins, & Crowder, 1974; Crowder, 1979). The second procedure scores position accuracy, which can be more complicated. Simply counting the number of transpositions is not acceptable if there are differences in item recall. One solution is to score the number of items recalled in correct position as a proportion of the number of items recalled regardless of position (Wickelgren, 1965; Poirier & Saint-Aubin, 1995; Saint-Aubin & Poirier, in press). There are important reasons for thinking about immediate serial recall in terms of item and position accuracy scoring. For example, the absence of phonological similarity effects under suppression or a brief retention interval has led a number of authors to suppose that phonemic traces only support recall for a few seconds. After that period, either decay or interference is assumed to reduce the integrity of the trace to levels where it cannot support recall (Baddeley, 1986; Tehan & Humphreys, 1995). If these assumptions are correct, the absence of phonemic representations would be manifest as a null effect of phonological similarity for both the item and accuracy scoring. An alternative way of accounting for the attenuation of similarity effects is to suggest that similarity might have opposite effects on item and Phonological similarity 5 accuracy scoring. That is, it is possible that similarity might facilitate item recall but lead to an increase in transposition errors. Thus, under suppression or a delay, the absence of similarity effects could be due to a beneficial effect of similarity at the item level masking a detrimental effect at the accuracy level. This second scenario might occur if rhyme categories result in a category cuing effect (Crowder, 1979; Huttenlocher & Newcombe, 1976; Poirier & SaintAubin, 1995). In both the long-term memory literature (Tulving & Pearlstone, 1966) and-term memory literature (Huttenlocher & Newcombe, 1976; Poirier & SaintAubin, 1995; Saint-Aubin & Poirier, in press) taxonomic category membership acts as an effective retrieval cue to facilitate recall. In the short-term domain this advantage is limited to item recall (Huttenlocher & Newcombe, 1976; Poirier & Saint-Aubin, 1995; Saint-Aubin & Poirier, in press). If taxonomic categories can serve as a retrieval cue, rhyme categories might also improve item recall in phonologically similar lists. In spite of the fundamental importance of the phonological similarity effect, only six studies have investigated phonological similarity effects at the item level. The results from these studies are equivocal. Sometimes a beneficial effect is observed (Gathercole, Gardiner, & Gregg, 1982; Wickelgren, 1965), sometimes, no effect is observed (Poirier & Saint-Aubin, 1996; Watkins et al., 1974); and some studies show a detrimental effect (Coltheart, 1993; Drewnowski, 1980) The dimension that appears most relevant for discriminating between these studies is the operationalisation of phonological similarity. The studies that demonstrated beneficial effects operationalised phonological similarity in terms of items from rhyme categories. Those that demonstrated a detrimental effect operationalised phonological similarity in terms of high phonemic overlap but no category membership (e.g. can mad cap man cad cat map mat). The one study Phonological similarity 6 (Watkins et al., 1974) that demonstrated a null effect used a mixture of rhyming and non-rhyming items in their similar lists. Given these findings, one might conclude that a beneficial effect of phonological similarity only occurs when rhyming items are used. In the following studies we investigated item and accuracy scoring of phonological similarity where similarity was operationalised in two distinct ways; either items from rhyme categories or items that had high phonemic overlap but did not come from any single rhyme category. The expectation was that phonological similarity effects would differ for the two types of lists. We expected that there would be differences in item recall between the two types of similar lists and that this would have an impact upon correct-in-position effects. We were not certain about differential order errors but expected both types of similarity to produce transposition errors. Experiment 1 Method Participants. Thirty-six undergraduates from the Northern Territory University volunteered to participate in this experiment. The participants represented a mix of students from a variety of disciplines, tertiary levels, and age groups. Materials. The materials used in this experiment are those used by Coltheart (1993), plus 5 additional words. Coltheart’s similar materials comprised 50 3-letter-'a' words. These included a limited set of eight final consonants (e.g. rat, map, tab, fad, can, gag, lax, dam). Coltheart’s similar items can be reorganised into 8 rhyme sets (i.e. at, ap, ab, ad, an, ag, ax, am). Because each ending set did not contain six items, five words were added to this pool: nag, fax, pam, rag, and max. The 50 dissimilar words in Coltheart’s set matched the similar words in word frequency. Phonological similarity 7 These two sets were used to create eight dissimilar, eight similar nonrhyming, and eight rhyming six-word lists that were sampled from the appropriate pool without replacement. Thus the words from the similar pool were sampled twice such that each word appeared in one similar and one rhyming list. A requirement of construction of the similar non-rhyming lists was that no items share a final consonant. Similarly, following random sampling, dissimilar items were rearranged across lists so that each list contained the minimum number of words that shared the same start or stop consonant. Essentially, these two sampling constraints were intended to maintain a relatively constant level of similarity and dissimilarity across conditions. The lists were presented in three blocks of eight trials. The order of the items within each list, the order of the trials within each block, and the order of the blocks within the experiment session were counterbalanced. Procedure. All subjects were individually tested, and assigned to the betweensubject conditions randomly such that 18 participants were assigned to each of the study conditions. Each trial began with a warning tone. Six words were then presented individually in the centre of a computer screen at the rate of one word per second. Immediately following the presentation of the last list item, a row of question marks was displayed as a recall prompt. Strict serial recall instructions were used: Participants were required to write the words in the order they were presented, in columns numbered one to six and to put a dash if they could not recall an item. The study conditions for each list differed for the two sets of participants. Half were required to articulate "the" as quickly as possible during presentation of list items. The other half remained silent during item presentation. Everyone attempted to recall the items under silent conditions. Results Phonological similarity 8 Table 1 summarises performance for similar non-rhyming, rhyming, and dissimilar trials in silent and suppressed conditions, collapsed across serial position for the three scoring procedures. 3*2 mixed-design analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were used to analyse the three sets of data. In this and all subsequent analyses, an alpha level of .05 was selected, unless specified otherwise. Correct-in-position performance was better in the silent than the suppression condition, F = (1,34), = 9.21, MSE = .08. A main effect of phonological similarity was also observed, F (2,68) = 16.02, MSE = .09. Post-hoc repeated-measures t-tests with sequential Bonferroni adjustments revealed that dissimilar lists were better recall than the similar non-rhyming lists, t (35) = 6.37, but there was no difference between the dissimilar and rhyming lists, t (34) = 1.33. Phonological similarity did not interact with the articulatory suppression, F (2,68) <1, MSE = .09. For item scoring an item was recalled as correct if it was produced anywhere in the output. Item recall was higher in the silent condition than the suppression condition, F (1,34) = 10.90, MSE = .06. A main effect was also observed for phonological similarity, F (2,68) = 67.49. MSE = .01. Bonferroni adjusted, post-hoc comparisons indicated that performance was higher in the rhyming condition than the dissimilar condition, t (35) = 5.15. Performance in the dissimilar condition was higher than performance in the similar non-rhyming condition, t (35) = 6.52. Phonological similarity did not interact with articulatory suppression, F (2,68)<1, MSE = .01. The measure we use to reflect order accuracy involves dividing the correct-inposition score by the item score. This produces the proportion of items in correct position as a function of items correctly recalled; the greater the number of transpositions the lower this proportion will be. Under these scoring conditions, the main effect for suppression was not significant, F (1,34) = 1.84, MSE = .05. A main Phonological similarity 9 effect of phonological similarity was observed, F (2, 68) = 29.91, MSE = .01. Bonferroni adjusted comparisons revealed better performance in the dissimilar condition than either the rhyming condition, t (35) = 7.21, or the similar non-rhyming condition, t (35) = 2.34. In addition, better performance emerged in the similar nonrhyming condition than the rhyming condition, t (35) = 4.74. There was no interaction between phonological similarity and articulatory suppression, F (2,66)<1, MSE = .01. Discussion With the correct-in-position scoring under silent conditions we replicate the detrimental effect of phonological similarity with the non-rhyming similar material. However, unlike previous research the phonological similarity effect was not attenuated under suppression for either type of list. We are not sure why this happened, but it may be a function of suppression being a between-subjects variable, rather than a within-subjects variable as it usually is. With item scoring, recall was best when the items in the list all rhymed. It looks as though membership of a rhyme category can act as a retrieval cue to facilitate item recall. Furthermore, when similar items do not rhyme, similarity has a detrimental effect upon item recall. Not only are the dissimilar lists well recalled, they are recalled accurately. This is not the case with both types of similar lists where a high number of transposition errors occur. The rhyming items are transposed more often than the nonrhyming similar. It is not clear whether this is a category effect or due to the fact that there is more phonemic overlap in the representations of the rhyming items than the non-rhyming items. Whatever the explanation, it is the case that the advantage that category membership produces with item information disappears with position accuracy scoring. Phonological similarity 10 In comparing recall of the two similar conditions to that of the dissimilar conditions, we are in a position to evaluate our original proposition that phonological similarity effects under item scoring depend upon the way in which phonological similarity is operationalised. In the current study the comparison between the dissimilar and the similar non-rhyming conditions represent a close replication of Coltheart’s experiment. She found that with these materials there was a similarity decrement. We replicate these results. However, when we take the same set of words but reorganise them into their rhyme categories, we find a facilitatory effect of similarity, as have others who have used lists of rhyming items (Gathercole et al., 1982; Wickelgren, 1965). It would seem that we have some evidence to explain the differences in similarity effects that have been observed in prior research. The absence of phonological similarity effects under silent conditions with the correct-in-position measure for the rhyming lists can be viewed as an item advantage for the rhyming lists being offset by an increased propensity for transpositions. With the non-rhyming similar lists we have a disadvantage for item recall coupled with increases in transpositions which translates into the phonological similarity decrement. The effects under suppression appear to emerge in much the same way. Both types of similar lists produce transposition errors, rhyming lists have an item advantage, and similar non-rhyming lists do not. Thus, similarity effects in correct-inposition scores appear critically dependent upon item recall. Additionally, these results imply that phonemic influences are still strong under suppression conditions. Consequently, those who have argued that suppression prevents a phonemic trace from being formed (Baddeley, 1986) may well be in error. Re-analysis of Tehan & Humphreys (1995; Experiment 1) Tehan and Humphreys (1995) made the same assumption about the effects of a brief, filled retention interval on the phonemic similarity effect. They (Tehan & Phonological similarity 11 Humphreys, 1995, Experiment 1) had their subjects study 4-item rhyming or dissimilar lists. Recall was tested immediately or after a 2-s retention interval filled with a verbal shadowing task. With the correct-in-position measure they found the usual detrimental effect of phonological similarity on an immediate test, but no difference in recall between rhyming and dissimilar lists on the delayed test. They suggested that the trace became degraded during the two-second interval such that it no longer supported recall. If this argument is correct, one would expect to find no differences on item or accuracy measures between rhyming and dissimilar lists. However, if the results of Experiment 1 are an indication, it is possible that effects under delayed conditions reflect a trade-off between item and order scoring. In Table 2 we present the original correct-in-position scores and a re-scoring of their data using the item and position accuracy procedures that were used in Experiment 1. An analysis of item scores indicated no similarity effect on an immediate test, F (1,19) = 2.02, MSE = .002, but on the delayed test, the rhyming words were much better recalled than the dissimilar words, F (1,19) = 13.84; MSE = .014. For the position accuracy measure, dissimilar words were more likely to be recalled in position than rhyming words on both immediate and delayed tests, F (1,19) = 21.95; MSE = .005, and F (1,19) = 30.26; MSE = .002, respectively. In short, our re-analysis of the Tehan and Humphreys (1995) data indicates that their interpretation of the delayed correct-in-position results was incorrect. The attenuation of the phonological similarity effect under delayed testing is the result of a cuing advantage for the rhyming lists in item recall offsetting the deleterious effects of increased transposition errors. The results of the suppression condition in Experiment 1 and the delayed condition of the Tehan and Humphreys experiments are identical for item and position accuracy scoring. More transpositions are made in the rhyming lists that the Phonological similarity 12 dissimilar lists but item recall is better in the rhyming lists than the dissimilar lists. However, in silent conditions, the similarity advantage in item scoring in Experiment 1 is absent in the Tehan and Humphreys experiment. Absolute levels of item recall are higher in the Tehan and Humphreys experiment in which 4-word lists were studied, than was the case in Experiment 1 where 6-word lists were studied. One possibility is that more forgetting might occur during recall of the long lists than the short lists (Cowan, 1993), such that substantial trace degradation is present for some items in the longer lists. Thus, similarity may facilitate item information with the longer lists in the same way that it does with suppression or a filled retention interval. To bridge the gap between 4-word and 6-word lists, in the next experiment we use 5word lists. Experiment 2 In this experiment we again studied serial recall of dissimilar, rhyming and similar non-rhyming lists. Recall was tested immediately or after a brief filled retention interval. Experiment 1 (and Tehan and Humphreys) used open word pools to construct the study lists. We wondered what effect a closed word pool might have on item and transposition effects. To this extent, we took Baddeley's (1966) materials that consist of two 8-item word pools: one containing dissimilar items and the other containing non-rhyming similar items. To this we added another 8-item word pool which consisted of eight items from a single rhyme category. Thus all trials in the experiment were constructed from these three word pools. Obviously, with such small word pools, items appeared more than once throughout the experiment. Our expectations were that we would replicate the principle findings of Experiment 1.

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

Re-Examining the Phonological Similarity Effect in Immediate Serial Recall: The Roles of Type of Similarity, Category Cueing, and Item Recall

Study of the phonological similarity effect (PSE) in immediate serial recall (ISR) has produced a conflicting body of results. Five experiments tested various theoretical ideas that together may help integrate these results. Experiments 1 and 2 tested alternative accounts that explain the effect of phonological similarity on item recall in terms of either feature overlap, linguistic structure, ...

متن کامل

Reexamining the phonological similarity effect in immediate serial recall: the roles of type of similarity, category cuing, and item recall.

Study of the phonological similarity effect (PSE) in immediate serial recall (ISR) has produced a conflicting body of results. Five experiments tested various theoretical ideas that together may help integrate these results. Experiments 1 and 2 tested alternative accounts that explain the effect of phonological similarity on item recall in terms of feature overlap, linguistic structure, or seri...

متن کامل

Theoretical implications of articulatory duration, phonological similarity, and phonological complexity in verbal working memory.

The phonological-loop model provides a prominent theoretical description of verbal working memory. According to it, serial recall accuracy should be inversely related to the articulatory duration and phonological similarity of verbal items in memorized sequences. Initial tests of these predictions by A. D. Baddeley and colleagues (e.g., A. D. Baddeley, N. Thomson, & M. Buchanan, 1975) appeared ...

متن کامل

Lexicality and phonological similarity: a challenge for the retrieval-based account of serial recall?

The retrieval-based account of serial recall (Saint-Aubin & Poirier, 2000) attributes lexicality, phonological similarity, and articulatory suppression effects to a process where long-term representations are used to reconstruct degraded phonological traces. Two experiments tested this assumption by manipulating these factors in the recall of four- and five-item lists of words and non-words. Le...

متن کامل

Age and redintegration in immediate memory and their relationship to task difficulty.

It is commonly assumed that as short-term memory tasks become more difficult, a transient phonological trace that supports recall loses its fidelity. Recall can still be achieved through a process called redintegration, where long-term phonological or lexical knowledge is used to reconstruct the memory trace. In the present research, we explored age-related differences in the redintegration pro...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2007